Stereotypes Can Harm Performance Of Older Adults On Cognitive And Physical Tasks | Nutrition Fit

0
307

[ad_1]

Summary: When older people feel personally stigmatized as being physically or cognitively deficient, they fail to live up to their actual abilities when tested, a new study reports.

Source: Georgia State University

When older adults are viewed as cognitively or physically impaired, they perform below their abilities on tasks, according to a recent review article by Sarah Barber, a psychology and gerontology researcher at Georgia State University.

Groups who are stigmatized—whether due to race, socioeconomic status or age—perform more poorly when they are faced with negative stereotypes, Barber said. She found expectations of others can play a powerful role in how well older adults perform on cognitive tasks and motor skills such as driving.

The phenomenon is known as “stereotype threat,” according to Barber. Her review article, published in the Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, looks at recent studies as well as those dating back to the mid-1990s, all of which show the power of this phenomenon.

“The concept was originally formulated to look at stereotypes around race,” Barber said, but the effect turned out to be much broader. She said it can affect older adults by affecting their memory, physical performance, driving abilities and even job satisfaction.

Older adults frequently encounter the challenge of stereotype threat at their physician’s office, where they routinely go for checkups, Barber said, and where they may sometimes be given cognitive tests as well. Research shows about 17 percent of individuals aged 50 and older experience stereotype threat at the doctor’s office, and about 8 percent worry their physician is negatively evaluating them because of their age.

This can lead older people to underperform on the cognitive tests they are given and can lead to greater distrust of physicians, greater dissatisfaction with healthcare services, poorer self-reported mental and physical health, and even higher rates of hypertension.

Just as important in stereotypes about age, Barber said, is negative self-evaluation, which she reviewed in a 2017 paper.

“People worry that there is truth to the negatives,” she said. “When they forget, they may worry they are on a slippery slope towards dementia and decline.”

This shows an older gentleman driving a car
These stereotype threat effects can also affect physical performance. Image credited to Georgia State University

That can be detrimental and actually lead to more forgetting. When conducting her own research on this topic, Barber said “I was struck by the negative things older adults would say about themselves, and I’d wonder how much better they might be performing if they weren’t so worried.”

These stereotype threat effects can also affect physical performance.

“Older adults are often stereotyped as being slow, weak, feeble and frail,” Barber said.

In lab studies, she said, stereotype threat can also lead to slower walking and weaker grip strength for older adults.

“We need to make people feel confident in their own abilities,” she said, “and feel that they will be respected no matter how they perform.”

Barber said older adults would also benefit from looking at their own attitudes about aging.

“Your own attitude about aging is highly predictive of your aging outcomes,” she said. “Those who have positive attitudes about aging live longer, have better memory function and recover more easily from illnesses.”

About this aging and psychology research news

Source: Georgia State University
Contact: Anna Varela – Georgia State University
Image: The image is credited to Georgia State University

Original Research: Closed access.
The Applied Implications of Age-Based Stereotype Threat for Older Adults” by Sarah Barber. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition


Abstract

See also

This shows question marks

The Applied Implications of Age-Based Stereotype Threat for Older Adults

Stereotype threat occurs when people feel concerned about the possibility of confirming, or being negatively judged by, a negative stereotype.

This review highlights the applied implications of this phenomenon for older adults. In clinical settings, older adults often feel that their physicians have negative expectations about their abilities because of their age. These feelings of age-based stereotype threat can increase older adults’ subjective cognitive complaints and impair their performance on mental status examinations.

Other research has shown that stereotype threat also adversely affects older adults’ physical performance, motor learning, and driving performance. In workplace settings, older adults who experience stereotype threat also report lower job satisfaction, poorer work-related mental health, and greater intentions to resign or retire.

Overall, this review provides evidence that the situational phenomenon of stereotype threat can affect older adults’ performance in a variety of applied settings, and this can contribute to age differences in performance.

[ad_2]

Source link